Skip to main content

Social Thinking in Psychology Course.

 INTRODUCTION

Why do people do horrible things? Slave owners and Nazis, any of the perpetrators of history's atrocities, how did they so successfully dehumanize other people for so long? At a smaller scale, how do bullies in the lunchroom manage to treat other kids with such cruelty and then go home and pet their dog and call their grandma and say happy birthday? Most of what we've been studying so far has focused on the individual. We've covered sub-fields of psychology like cognitive, personality, and clinical psychology, which tend to address the phenomena contained within a single person's mind. But there's also social psychology, which focuses on the power of the situation. It examines how we think about, influence, and relate to one another in certain conditions, and it's better equipped to answer this question about people doing horrible things. 

Social psychology can not only give us some of the tools we need to understand why people behave brutally, it can also help us understand why we sometimes act heroically. Like, why did Jean Valjean reveal his true identity to save some stranger from being tried in his place? Why did Nazi Oskar Schindler risk his own hide to save over a thousand Jewish people? What made Darth Vader throw the Emperor down that hole, even as he was being electrocuted? I can't say there are any easy answers about either humanity's greatness or its horribleness. Certainly, there aren't any that we can find in the next ten minutes. But we can point ourselves in the right direction, and it starts with social thinking. When we're trying to understand why people act like villains or heroes, one of the things we're really asking is, did they do what they did because of their personality or their situation? 



ATTRIBUTION THEORY

Austrian psychologist Fritz Haider began plumbing the depths of this question in the 1920s when he was developing what's now known as the attribution theory. This theory simply suggests that we can explain someone's behavior by crediting either their stable, enduring traits, also known as their disposition or the situation at hand. And we tend to attribute people's behavior to either one or the other. Sounds pretty simple, but it can be surprisingly hard to tell whether someone's behavior is dispositional or situational. Say you see Bruno at a party, and he's acting like a wallflower all night. You might assume that he just has a shy personality, but maybe he doesn't. Maybe he'd ordinarily be reenacting all the moves from Footloose at this party. But on this night, he had a twisted ankle or a headache, or he'd just seen his ex with somebody new those are all situational explanations. 

Overestimating the forces of personality while underestimating the power of the situation is called the fundamental attribution error. As you can imagine, making this kind of error can really end up warping your opinion of another person and lead to false snap judgments. This might not be such a big deal when it comes to Bruno and his awesome dance moves, but according to one study of college students, 7 in 10 women report that men have misread their polite friendliness, which would be appropriate for the situation, as a sexual come-on. We choose how we explain other people's behavior every day, and what we choose to believe can have big consequences. For example, our political views will likely be strongly influenced by whether we decide to attribute poverty or homelessness to personal dispositions, like being lazy and looking for a handout, or social circumstances, like lack of education and opportunity. 

These attitudes can in turn affect our actions, Activists and politicians know this well, and they can use it to their advantage to persuade people in different ways. In the late 1970s and 80s, psychologists Richard Petty and John Cacioppo developed a dual process theory of understanding how persuasion works. The first part of their model is known as the central route persuasion, and it involves calling on basic thinking and reasoning to convince people. This is what's at work when interested people focus on the evidence and arguments at hand and are persuaded by the actual content of the message. So when you're watching a political debate, you might be persuaded by a candidate's particular policies, positions, or voting history. That is, the stuff they're actually saying. 

But we all know that persuasion involves more than that. There's also peripheral route persuasion at work. This influences people by way of incidental cues, like a speaker's physical attractiveness or personal relatability. There's not a lot of hard thinking going on here. It's more of a gut reaction. So you might decide to vote for a particular candidate because you think they're cute or because they're from your hometown. Peripheral route persuasion happens more readily when you're not paying a ton of attention, which is why billboards and television ads can be scarily effective. So that's how politicians and advertisers and maybe bosses and teachers and pushy friends try to change our behavior by changing our attitudes. But it turns out that the reverse is true, too. 

Our attitudes can be affected by our behaviors. You might have heard of the phrase, fake it till you make it, meaning if you smile when you're actually sad, the act of smiling may carry you through an attitude change until you actually feel better. Sometimes we can manipulate ourselves this way, but it's also an incredibly effective method that people use to persuade each other. It generally works best in increments through what psychologists call the foot-in-the-door phenomenon. People tend to more readily comply with a big request after they've first agreed to smaller, more innocuous requests. Like Darth Vader didn't just go from go get a mannequin to Dark Lord overnight. He was slowly enticed to the dark side by a series of escalating actions and attitude changes. 

Do this favor for me, now run this errand, now kill these padawans, now blow up a planet! What started as small actions went on to become big ones, subtly transforming Vader's beliefs about himself, and others. There's plenty of experimental evidence that moral action really does strengthen moral convictions, just as amoral action strengthens amoral attitudes. And there is perhaps no better example of this than the Stanford Prison Experiment. Back in 1971, Stanford psych professor Philip Zimbardo and his team put an ad in the local paper looking for volunteers to participate in a 14-day experiment. After screening around 70 applicants, 24 male college students were deemed physically and mentally fit enough to participate in the study. For their troubles, they'd each be given 15 bucks a day. 

The participants didn't know the exact nature of the experiment, just that it involved a fake prison situation, and with a coin flip, half were randomly deemed prisoners and the other half guards. The guards were told that it was the prisoners' behavior that was being studied. The prisoners weren't told much of anything, aside from that they'd been arrested and taken to prison. Other than that, neither group had many specific instructions. Zimbardo wanted to observe how each party adapted to their roles, and so On a quiet Sunday summer morning in Palo Alto, real cops swooped in and arrested the prisoners in their homes under charges of robbery. They were frisked, handcuffed, and read their rights. Back at the station, they were formally booked and then blindfolded in a holding cell wearing only hospital gowns. 

The researchers had taken great care to make sure that the setting was extremely realistic, which is one reason why they used real cops in the arrest before handing the prisoners over to the fake guards. And it took no time at all for this role-playing to become really, really real. The initial trauma of the humiliation of the arrest, the booking, strip-searching, and waiting immediately kicked off a loss of identity in the prisoners. A few prisoners only made it through the first night before they became too emotionally distressed and had to be released. Things only went downhill from there. Though the guards could act any way they wanted, as long as they didn't physically hurt anyone, encounters quickly became cruel, hostile, and dehumanizing. Guards hurled insults and commands, referred to the prisoners only by number, and put some of them in solitary confinement. Prisoners started breaking down, others rebelled, and still others became passively resigned as if they deserved to be treated so badly. 

Things got bad enough that the experiment ended after only six days, causing relief in the fake prisoners, while interestingly leaving some fake guards feeling angry. Luckily, everyone involved bounced back to normal once out of the prison setting. All of those negative moods and abusive behaviors were situational. And that fact reinforced the important concept that the power of a given situation can easily override individual personality differences. Although it would never fly by today's ethical standards, Zimbardo's famous study remains influential today because it sheds such a harsh light on the nature of power and corruption. 

And yet, People differ. Many people succumb and become compliant in terrible situations, but not everyone does. Lots of people risked their lives to hide Jewish people in World War II, help runaway slaves along the Underground Railroad, keep Tutsi refugees safe during the Rwandan genocide, or generally refuse to comply or participate in actions they didn't believe in. Some people can and do resist turning to the dark side, even when it seems like everyone around them is going mad. And yet, the fact is, these people tend to be in the minority. So why? Why does it seem so easy to rationalize a negative action or attitude, and so hard to muster the positive ones? One partial explanation comes from American social psychologist Leon Festinger's theory of cognitive dissonance. 

It's one of the most important concepts in psychology. Festinger's theory begins with the notion that we experience discomfort, or dissonance when our thoughts, beliefs, or behaviors are inconsistent with each other. Basically, We don't like to confuse ourselves. For example, if Bruno is generally considered a peaceful person, but finds himself suddenly punching at his friend over a vendor bender, he's likely experiencing some level of cognitive dissonance. So, by Festinger's thinking, Bruno might relieve this tension by actually modifying his beliefs to match the actions he's already committed. Like telling himself, turns out I'm not such a nice guy after all. Maybe I'm actually a bully. On the other hand, he might resolve his internal tension by changing how he thinks about the situation. 

He might still think of himself as a peaceful person but realized that an unusual situation led to an unusual action. Like, he'd had a bad day, and it was his mom's new car, or his friend was just really asking for it. So he can keep being the ordinarily peaceful guy he was before. It's kind of an inverted fundamental attribution error if you think about it, attributing a person's actions mainly to the situation instead of his personality. The point is that this mismatch between what we do and who we think we are induces tension, and cognitive dissonance, and that we tend to want to resolve that tension. That's part of what turns an Anakin into a Darth Vader, and then, if we're lucky, back into an Anakin. 

Today, you learned that social psychology studies how people relate to each other. We discussed Fritz Heider's attribution theory and fundamental attribution error. You also learned how attitudes can affect actions, like through the dual process theory of persuasion, and also how behavior can change attitudes, like through the foot-in-the-door phenomenon. The Stanford Prison Experiment illustrated how a situation can override individual personality differences, while Leon Festinger's theory of cognitive dissonance explained how we ease the tension between conflicting thoughts and actions.

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

The Bobo Beatdown: Experiment Beating up an Inanimate Clown

The Bobo Doll Experiment It's 1961. You're wandering around Stanford University looking for a sandwich or something when you happen to walk by a particular room in a particular lab and see something a little unnerving. Namely, you find a woman punching an inflatable clown named Bobo in the neck. over and over in its neck. This was the lab of legendary psychologist Albert Bandura, and in 1961 he was studying one of the most important phenomena in psychology. See, while that woman was throttling that big inflatable clown, a child was watching her. After about ten minutes of observing this clown-beating display, the kid was taken to a room full of fun toys. which were soon taken away, and then the frustrated kid was left alone with Bobo, and Bandura watched what happened.   Social Cognitive Learning. And yeah, what happened was kinda scary. Kids who watched the woman beating the clown were much more likely to mimic her aggression, kicking, punching, throwing, and even attempting t

The Chemical Mind: Neuron.

  The Chemical Mind Say it's late at night. You're home alone, drifting off to sleep, just entering that dream about Fritos, and then suddenly there's a banging at the door. Suddenly, you're wide awake, and it feels like your heart's gonna explode. You jump up, ready to run out the back door, possibly grab a Phillips head screwdriver and stab it into the darkness until it sticks into something. Now, whether it's a weeping angel or your neighbor looking to borrow a can of beans, it doesn't really matter, because when you heard that sudden noise, your startled brain released an icy typhoon of chemicals, and everything that's now going through your mind, like your urge to flee, your urge to defend yourself, that internal debate about whether weeping angels are even real, and, well, where's the cat?  All that. It's just a result of those chemicals. Our brains and our nervous systems and the substances they produce and are always bathed in are amazing

How to Train a Brain: You've Heard of Pavlov's Bell

  INTRODUCTION:Ivan Pavlov So the name Ivan Pavlov rings a bell. It's because his experiments are among the most famous in the history of psychology. His work contributed to the foundation of the behaviorist school of thought that viewed psychology as an empirically rigorous science focused on observable behaviors and not unobservable internal mental processes. Even though today we view psychology as the science of both behavior and mental processes, Pavlov's influence was tremendous. His research helped pave the path for more experimental rigor and behavioral research right up to the present day. Born in 1849 in Russia, Pavlov was never much for psychology. After giving up on his original aspirations to become a Russian Orthodox priest like his father, he instead earned a medical degree and spent nearly 20 years studying the digestive system, earning Russia's first Nobel Prize in his mid-50s for his research expanding our understanding of how stomachs worked.  procedures w